120 Participants Needed

Vocal-Social Reinforcement for Infant Speech Learning With Hearing Loss

NV
SW
MN
Overseen ByMartin Nunez Rivera, B.S.
Age: < 18
Sex: Any
Trial Phase: Academic
Sponsor: University of Southern California
No Placebo GroupAll trial participants will receive the active study treatment (no placebo)

Trial Summary

What is the purpose of this trial?

The goal of this study is to investigate the role of social factors on speech learning, including production and perception, in infants ranging in age from \~7-18 months. Infants have either typical hearing or sensorineural hearing loss. The main prediction of the study is that social reinforcement will engender improvements in vocal learning above and beyond gains in hearing in infants with hearing loss. As part of this study: * The parent and infant engage in a free play session in the playroom while the investigator cues the parent to say simple nonsense words; * Infants hear playback of the same words during a second phase.

Do I need to stop my current medications for this trial?

The trial protocol does not specify whether you need to stop taking your current medications.

What data supports the idea that Vocal-Social Reinforcement for Infant Speech Learning With Hearing Loss is an effective treatment?

The available research shows that infants can quickly learn to increase their vocalizations when given positive reinforcement, even without social interaction. In a study, infants increased their vocalizations in just five minutes when they received visual rewards. This suggests that Vocal-Social Reinforcement can be effective in encouraging speech development in infants, including those with hearing loss. While the research does not directly compare this treatment to others for hearing loss, it highlights the potential of using reinforcement to improve vocal behavior, which could be beneficial for infants at risk of poor language skills.12345

What safety data exists for Vocal-Social Reinforcement therapy for infants with hearing loss?

The provided research does not directly address the safety data for Vocal-Social Reinforcement therapy or its variants. The studies focus on visual reinforcement audiometry and its applications, as well as reinforcement techniques in different contexts, but do not provide specific safety evaluations for the therapy in question.12356

Is the treatment Vocal-Social Reinforcement promising for helping infants with hearing loss learn to speak?

Yes, Vocal-Social Reinforcement is promising because it helps infants learn to increase their vocalizations through positive feedback, which can be beneficial for infants with hearing loss who are slower to develop spoken vocabulary.35789

Research Team

SW

Sarah W Bottjer, Ph.D.

Principal Investigator

University of Southern California

Eligibility Criteria

This study is for infants about 7-18 months old with either typical hearing or sensorineural hearing loss. They must have at least one parent who speaks English or Spanish and can participate in the study. Infants not exposed to these languages, or without a participating parent, cannot join.

Inclusion Criteria

My child is under 24 months old and needs a follow-up visit.
Infants with normal hearing, hearing loss, or hearing loss improved with a hearing aid or cochlear implant can participate.
I have a parent at home who speaks English or Spanish and can join the study.
See 1 more

Exclusion Criteria

Infants who do not have a parent who can participate in the study will be excluded (Caregivers who are not parents will not be eligible to participate in the study)
My infant is not exposed to English or Spanish at home.

Treatment Details

Interventions

  • Vocal-Social Reinforcement
Trial OverviewThe trial examines how social interactions affect speech learning in infants. It involves parents playing with their child and saying nonsense words, which are then played back to the infant to see if it influences vocal learning.
Participant Groups
2Treatment groups
Experimental Treatment
Active Control
Group I: ExperimentalExperimental Treatment1 Intervention
Parents are instructed to say nonsense words in response to infant babbles with a conserved phonological form as infant plays.
Group II: ControlActive Control1 Intervention
Parents are instructed to say nonsense words at random times with a conserved phonological form as infant plays.

Find a Clinic Near You

Who Is Running the Clinical Trial?

University of Southern California

Lead Sponsor

Trials
956
Recruited
1,609,000+

Cornell University

Collaborator

Trials
179
Recruited
14,090,000+

National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD)

Collaborator

Trials
377
Recruited
190,000+

Findings from Research

In a study involving 14 normal-hearing infants aged 7-16 months, both video visual reinforcement audiometry (VVRA) and conventional visual reinforcement audiometry (CVRA) showed similar response patterns, indicating that infants respond consistently to both methods.
The findings suggest that VVRA is a viable option for hearing tests in infants, but further research is needed to assess its effectiveness in infants with hearing loss before clinical use.
A comparison of video versus conventional visual reinforcement in 7- to 16-month-old infants.Lowery, KJ., von Hapsburg, D., Plyler, EL., et al.[2019]
Visual reinforcement audiometry (VRA) proved to be an effective testing method for low-functioning children with developmental ages of approximately 10 months or higher, allowing for accurate threshold assessments.
The study found that 52% of the children tested were referred for medical evaluation based on VRA thresholds and tympanometry results, highlighting its utility in identifying hearing issues and potential speech-sound discrimination challenges.
Application of visual reinforcement audiometry (VRA) to low-functioning children.Thompson, G., Wilson, WR., Moore, JM.[2019]
Infants can learn to increase their vocalizations in response to positive visual reinforcement, as demonstrated in a study where they controlled the appearance of shapes on an iPad by vocalizing more, marking the first evidence of this behavior without social interaction.
This research lays the groundwork for exploring how early vocalizations relate to language development and suggests potential clinical applications for encouraging vocal practice in infants at risk for language delays.
"Did I just do that?"-Six-month-olds learn the contingency between their vocalizations and a visual reward in 5 minutes.Keren-Portnoy, T., Daffern, H., DePaolis, RA., et al.[2022]

References

A comparison of video versus conventional visual reinforcement in 7- to 16-month-old infants. [2019]
Application of visual reinforcement audiometry (VRA) to low-functioning children. [2019]
"Did I just do that?"-Six-month-olds learn the contingency between their vocalizations and a visual reward in 5 minutes. [2022]
Language development in infants with hearing loss: Benefits of infant-directed speech. [2022]
The Effects of a Delay of Noncontingent Reinforcement during a Pairing Procedure in the Development of Stimulus Control of Automatically Reinforced Vocalizations. [2021]
Animated toys versus video reinforcement in 16-24-month-old children in a clinical setting. [2019]
Vocalizations of infants with hearing loss compared with infants with normal hearing: Part I--phonetic development. [2022]
Three procedures for increasing vocal response to therapist prompt in infants and children with Down syndrome. [2006]
Differential reinforcement of other-than-vocalization as a control procedure in the conditioning of infant vocalization rate. [2019]